Since I haven’t read this and remember little of my college exposure to Spinoza, I was mostly interested in the remarks about species adaption to joy, pain, well-being and sorrow as they contribute to survival of the species, whether that’s far in the future or as we try to adapt and absorb what we are accepting and rejecting in current world events. – Karen

The Neurologist and the Philosopher

A prominent neurologist traces much of what modern neuroscience is learning back to a 17th-century philosopher

By Erica Goode in Scientific American, February 10, 2003

 

Returning to his hotel in the Hague on a wet, wind-battered day in 1999, Antonio Damasio could not resist telling the doorman that he had just come from a visit to Spinoza's house. "You mean ... the philosopher?" the doorman responded after a pause. "They don't speak much of him, these days."

In fact, for most people, the author of The Ethics and other tracts is little more than a dimly remembered figure from a college textbook. Damasio, a prominent neurologist and the author of two previous popular books on emotions and the brain, sets out to redress this state of affairs in Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling Brain. Specifically, he would like to show that the philosopher, who took an active interest in the physics, astronomy and mathematics of his time, anticipated much of what neuroscientists are now learning about the human brain and, in particular, about the biological underpinnings of feelings and even consciousness itself.

The result is a volume that is by turns engaging and abstruse, elegant and disorganized, and that may prove daunting in some places to those without a passing acquaintance with neurophysiology. Blending the findings of recent studies and the case histories of brain-damaged patients, Damasio presents his own vision of how the thousands of small puzzle pieces that brain scientists are busily gathering might fit together. He speculates, for example, that feelings--joy, pain, well-being, sorrow--not only contribute to the survival of the species but impel humans toward ethical behavior and cooperative social organization. At the same time, in passages that deconstruct Spinoza's writings and discuss his life, he presses the philosopher's case.

It is no coincidence, Damasio believes, that Albert Einstein and other luminaries of the scientific world felt a kinship with Spinoza, a Dutch-born Jew of Portuguese descent, whose writings were deemed heretical and banned throughout Europe for almost a century after his death. The philosopher may have written mostly about religion and political structure, but he thought like a scientist.

For example, Damasio argues, in asserting that mind and body were inseparable, made "of the very same substance," Spinoza sensed that mental processes--thoughts, memories, emotions--were dependent on the neurophysiology of the brain, although he could know nothing of the chattering of neurons or the flow of neurotransmitters. Nevertheless, the philosopher, Damasio contends, intuited the role of feelings in paving the way for a conscious self. That role, in the neurologist's view, is gradually being supported by studies suggesting that certain brain areas are responsible for a constant monitoring of the body's overall state, registering the impact of events both external (an absence of food, the death of a loved one, a potential mugger on a street corner) and internal (an infection, the memory of a pleasant afternoon, embarrassment over a social misstep). What emerges from this global temperature reading are feelings, mental activity that stands apart from the raw data on which the reading itself is based. Spinoza, who stated that the "human mind is the very idea or knowledge of the human body," hinted at this concept, Damasio believes.

"I am convinced that mental processes are grounded in the brain's mappings of the body, collections of neural patterns that portray responses to events that cause emotions and feelings," he writes. The push to maximize a sense of well-being and to avoid pain and other feelings may have evolved as a strategy to help the organism survive. Spinoza certainly endorses the notion that the best way to combat a negative feeling is to overpower it with a positive feeling based in reason, a recipe for well-being that the author spends the last part of the book considering.

Scientists who write books come in two varieties: those who are cautious, reluctant to stray beyond the data before them, and those who are bold and synthetic, using what is known as a springboard for journeys into unproved theory. Damasio, whom some have accused of leaping ahead of what scientists actually know in order to construct convincing narratives, obviously belongs to the latter group. Some readers will fault him for it; others will see it as a strength. It is through such speculative leaps, after all, that understanding often advances.

Those who have read Damasio's popular works Descartes' Error and The Feeling of What Happens will find much that is already familiar. Still, Looking for Spinoza is compelling, in part because it so strongly conveys the feel of a personal expedition: the neurologist sifts through what is known about the philosopher's life as if pursuing a lost relative. How did he live? What did he read? Was he content during his exile from family and community? 

 

Damasio finds some answers and imagines others. Spinoza, in the author's view, was admirable in his bravery, kind in his later years, and likable in many ways but unyielding and strange in others. He was remarkable in his ability to adapt his life to the consequences of his exile. "In his own terms he succeeded," Damasio writes. The neurologist clearly hopes to do the same.

 

Erica Goode writes about human behavior for the New York Times

 

Scientificamerican.com

 

Reply via email to