Freud's Theory of Unconscious Conflict Linked to Anxiety Symptoms

ScienceDaily (June 16, 2012) — A link between unconscious conflicts and 
conscious anxiety disorder symptoms have been shown, lending empirical support 
to psychoanalysis.

An experiment that Sigmund Freud could never have imagined 100 years ago may 
help lend scientific support for one of his key theories, and help connect it 
with current neuroscience.
June 16 at the 101st Annual Meeting of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 
a University of Michigan professor who has spent decades applying scientific 
methods to the study of psychoanalysis will present new data supporting a 
causal link between the psychoanalytic concept known as unconscious conflict, 
and the conscious symptoms experienced by people with anxiety disorders such as 
phobias.
Howard Shevrin, Ph.D., emeritus professor of psychology in the U-M Medical 
School's Department of Psychiatry, will present data from experiments performed 
in U-M's Ormond and Hazel Hunt Laboratory.
The research involved 11 people with anxiety disorders who each received a 
series of psychoanalytically oriented diagnostic sessions conducted by a 
psychoanalyst.
>From these interviews the psychoanalysts inferred what underlying unconscious 
>conflict might be causing the person's anxiety disorder. Words capturing the 
>nature of the unconscious conflict were then selected from the interviews and 
>used as stimuli in the laboratory. They also selected words related to each 
>patient's experience of anxiety disorder symptoms. Although these words 
>differed from patient to patient, results showed that they functioned in the 
>same way.
These verbal stimuli were presented subliminally at one thousandth of a second, 
and supraliminally at 30 milliseconds. A control category of stimuli was added 
that had no relationship to the unconscious conflict or anxiety symptom. While 
the stimuli were presented to the patients, scalp electrodes record the brain 
responses to them.
In a previous experiment Shevrin had demonstrated that time-frequency features, 
a type of brain activity, showed that patients grouped the unconscious conflict 
stimuli together only when they were presented subliminally. But the conscious 
symptom-related stimuli showed the reverse pattern -- brain activity was better 
grouped together when patients viewed those words supraliminally.
"Only when the unconscious conflict words were presented unconsciously could 
the brain see them as connected," Shevrin notes. "What the analysts put 
together from the interview session made sense to the brain only unconsciously."
However, the experimental design in this first experiment did not allow for 
directly comparing the effect of the unconscious conflict stimuli on the 
conscious symptom stimuli.
To obtain evidence for that next level, the unconscious conflict stimuli were 
presented immediately prior to the conscious symptom stimuli and a new 
measurement was made, of the brain's own alpha wave frequency, at 8-13 cycles 
per second, that had been shown to inhibit various cognitive functions.
Highly significant correlations, suggesting an inhibitory effect, were obtained 
when the amount of alpha generated by the unconscious conflict stimuli were 
correlated with the amount of alpha associated with the conscious symptom alpha 
-- but only when the unconscious conflict stimuli were presented subliminally. 
No results were obtained when control stimuli replaced the symptom words. The 
fact that these findings are a function of inhibition suggests that from a 
psychoanalytic standpoint that repression might be involved.
"These results create a compelling case that unconscious conflicts cause or 
contribute to the anxiety symptoms the patient is experiencing," says Shevrin, 
who also holds an emeritus position in the Department of Psychology in U-M's 
College of Literature, Science and the Arts. "These findings and the 
interdisciplinary methods used -- which draw on psychoanalysis, cognitive 
psychology, and neuroscience -- demonstrate that it is possible to develop an 
interdisciplinary science drawing upon psychoanalytic theory."
He notes that a prominent critic of psychoanalysis and Freudian theory, Adolf 
Grunbaum, Ph.D., professor of the philosophy of science at the University of 
Pittsburgh, has expressed satisfaction that the new results, when added to 
previous evidence, show that fundamental psychoanalytic concepts can indeed be 
tested in empirical ways.
For more than 40 years, Shevrin has led a team that has pushed at the 
boundaries between the disciplines of neuroscience, cognitive psychology, and 
psychoanalysis, looking for evidence that Freudian concepts such as the 
unconscious and repression could be documented through physical measures of 
brain activity. His work has explored the territory where neurobiology, 
thoughts, emotions and behavior meet.
In 1968 he published the first report of brain responses to unconscious visual 
stimuli in Science, thus providing strong objective evidence for the existence 
of the unconscious at a time when most scientists were skeptical of Freud's 
ideas. In that same study, he showed that unconscious perceptions are processed 
in different ways from conscious perceptions, a finding consistent with Freud's 
views on how the unconscious works.
In recent years, exchanges between Grunbaum and Shevrin explored the nature of 
the evidence for the existence and impact of unconscious conflicts. In a 1992 
publication, the first study referred to, Grunbaum agreed that Shevrin had 
obtained objective brain based evidence for the existence of unconscious 
conflict, but Grunbaum noted that he had not shown that these conflicts caused 
psychiatric symptoms. His response to being informed of the new findings was an 
email stating: "I am satisfied."

http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2012/06/120616145531.htm

Reply via email to