Fw: [Mind and Brain] News: Brain-to-brain interface allows transmission oftactile and motor information between rats

2013-03-01 Thread Roger Clough



- Have received the following content - 
Sender: Robert Karl Stonjek 
Receiver: Psychiatry-Research,Cognitive NeuroScience,Mind and Brain 
Time: 2013-03-01, 00:21:49
Subject: [Mind and Brain] News: Brain-to-brain interface allows transmission 
oftactile and motor information between rats


  


Brain-to-brain interface allows transmission of tactile and motor information 
between rats
February 28th, 2013 in Neuroscience 
Enlarge

Researchers have electronically linked the brains of pairs of rats for the 
first time, enabling them to communicate directly to solve simple behavioral 
puzzles. Credit: Duke University Medical Center
Researchers have electronically linked the brains of pairs of rats for the 
first time, enabling them to communicate directly to solve simple behavioral 
puzzles. A further test of this work successfully linked the brains of two 
animals thousands of miles apart?ne in Durham, N.C., and one in Natal, Brazil.
The results of these projects suggest the future potential for linking multiple 
brains to form what the research team is calling an organic computer, which 
could allow sharing of motor and sensory information among groups of animals. 
The study was published Feb. 28, 2013, in the journal Scientific Reports.
Our previous studies with brain-machine interfaces had convinced us that the 
rat brain was much more plastic than we had previously thought, said Miguel 
Nicolelis, M.D., PhD, lead author of the publication and professor of 
neurobiology at Duke University School of Medicine. In those experiments, the 
rat brain was able to adapt easily to accept input from devices outside the 
body and even learn how to process invisible infrared light generated by an 
artificial sensor. So, the question we asked was, 'if the brain could 
assimilate signals from artificial sensors, could it also assimilate 
information input from sensors from a different body?'
To test this hypothesis, the researchers first trained pairs of rats to solve a 
simple problem: to press the correct lever when an indicator light above the 
lever switched on, which rewarded the rats with a sip of water. They next 
connected the two animals' brains via arrays of microelectrodes inserted into 
the area of the cortex that processes motor information.
One of the two rodents was designated as the encoder animal. This animal 
received a visual cue that showed it which lever to press in exchange for a 
water reward. Once this encoder rat pressed the right lever, a sample of its 
brain activity that coded its behavioral decision was translated into a pattern 
of electrical stimulation that was delivered directly into the brain of the 
second rat, known as the decoder animal.
The decoder rat had the same types of levers in its chamber, but it did not 
receive any visual cue indicating which lever it should press to obtain a 
reward. Therefore, to press the correct lever and receive the reward it craved, 
the decoder rat would have to rely on the cue transmitted from the encoder via 
the brain-to-brain interface.
The researchers then conducted trials to determine how well the decoder animal 
could decipher the brain input from the encoder rat to choose the correct 
lever. The decoder rat ultimately achieved a maximum success rate of about 70 
percent, only slightly below the possible maximum success rate of 78 percent 
that the researchers had theorized was achievable based on success rates of 
sending signals directly to the decoder rat's brain.
Importantly, the communication provided by this brain-to-brain interface was 
two-way. For instance, the encoder rat did not receive a full reward if the 
decoder rat made a wrong choice. The result of this peculiar contingency, said 
Nicolelis, led to the establishment of a behavioral collaboration between the 
pair of rats.
We saw that when the decoder rat committed an error, the encoder basically 
changed both its brain function and behavior to make it easier for its partner 
to get it right, Nicolelis said. The encoder improved the signal-to-noise 
ratio of its brain activity that represented the decision, so the signal became 
cleaner and easier to detect. And it made a quicker, cleaner decision to choose 
the correct lever to press. Invariably, when the encoder made those 
adaptations, the decoder got the right decision more often, so they both got a 
better reward.
In a second set of experiments, the researchers trained pairs of rats to 
distinguish between a narrow or wide opening using their whiskers. If the 
opening was narrow, they were taught to nose-poke a water port on the left side 
of the chamber to receive a reward; for a wide opening, they had to poke a port 
on the right side.
The researchers then divided the rats into encoders and decoders. The decoders 
were trained to associate stimulation pulses with the left reward poke as the 
correct choice, and an absence of pulses with the right reward poke as correct. 
During trials in which the encoder 

Re: Fw: [Mind and Brain] News: Brain-to-brain interface allows transmission oftactile and motor information between rats

2013-03-01 Thread Telmo Menezes
I was going to send that article to annoy Craig but then decided to leave
him in alone :)


On Fri, Mar 1, 2013 at 1:47 PM, Roger Clough rclo...@verizon.net wrote:




 - Have received the following content -
 *Sender:* Robert Karl Stonjek ston...@ozemail.com.au
 *Receiver:* Psychiatry-Research,Cognitive NeuroScience,Mind and 
 Brainpsychiatry-resea...@yahoogroups.com,cognitiveneurosciencefo...@yahoogroups.com,mindbr...@yahoogroups.com
 *Time:* 2013-03-01, 00:21:49
 *Subject:* [Mind and Brain] News: Brain-to-brain interface allows
 transmission oftactile and motor information between rats



 **
 http://medicalxpress.com/
  Brain-to-brain interface allows transmission of tactile and motor
 information between ratsFebruary 28th, 2013 in Neuroscience
 [image: Brain-to-brain interface allows transmission of tactile and motor
 information between 
 rats]Enlargehttp://s.ph-cdn.com/newman/gfx/news/hires/2013/braintobrain.jpg

 *Researchers have electronically linked the brains of pairs of rats for
 the first time, enabling them to communicate directly to solve simple
 behavioral puzzles. Credit: Duke University Medical Center*

 *Researchers have electronically linked the brains of pairs of rats for
 the first time, enabling them to communicate directly to solve simple
 behavioral puzzles. A further test of this work successfully linked the
 brains of two animals thousands of miles apartę¢ ne in Durham, N.C., and one
 in Natal, Brazil.*

 The results of these projects suggest the future potential for linking
 multiple brains to form what the research team is calling an organic
 computer, which could allow sharing of motor and sensory information among
 groups of animals. The study was published Feb. 28, 2013, in the journal 
 *Scientific
 Reports*.

 Our previous studies with brain-machine interfaces had convinced us that
 the rat brain was much more plastic than we had previously thought, said
 Miguel Nicolelis, M.D., PhD, lead author of the publication and professor
 of neurobiology at Duke University School of Medicine. In those
 experiments, the rat brain was able to adapt easily to accept input from
 devices outside the body and even learn how to process invisible infrared
 light generated by an artificial sensor. So, the question we asked was, 'if
 the brain could assimilate signals from artificial sensors, could it also
 assimilate information input from sensors from a different body?'

 To test this hypothesis, the researchers first trained pairs of rats to
 solve a simple problem: to press the correct lever when an indicator light
 above the lever switched on, which rewarded the rats with a sip of water.
 They next connected the two animals' brains via arrays of microelectrodes
 inserted into the area of the cortex that processes motor information.

 One of the two rodents was designated as the encoder animal. This animal
 received a visual cue that showed it which lever to press in exchange for a
 water reward. Once this encoder rat pressed the right lever, a sample of
 its brain activity that coded its behavioral decision was translated into a
 pattern of electrical stimulation that was delivered directly into the
 brain of the second rat, known as the decoder animal.

 The decoder rat had the same types of levers in its chamber, but it did
 not receive any visual cue indicating which lever it should press to obtain
 a reward. Therefore, to press the correct lever and receive the reward it
 craved, the decoder rat would have to rely on the cue transmitted from the
 encoder via the brain-to-brain interface.

 The researchers then conducted trials to determine how well the decoder
 animal could decipher the brain input from the encoder rat to choose the
 correct lever. The decoder rat ultimately achieved a maximum success rate
 of about 70 percent, only slightly below the possible maximum success rate
 of 78 percent that the researchers had theorized was achievable based on
 success rates of sending signals directly to the decoder rat's brain.

 Importantly, the communication provided by this brain-to-brain interface
 was two-way. For instance, the encoder rat did not receive a full reward if
 the decoder rat made a wrong choice. The result of this peculiar
 contingency, said Nicolelis, led to the establishment of a behavioral
 collaboration between the pair of rats.

 We saw that when the decoder rat committed an error, the encoder
 basically changed both its brain function and behavior to make it easier
 for its partner to get it right, Nicolelis said. The encoder improved the
 signal-to-noise ratio of its brain activity that represented the decision,
 so the signal became cleaner and easier to detect. And it made a quicker,
 cleaner decision to choose the correct lever to press. Invariably, when the
 encoder made those adaptations, the decoder got the right decision more
 often, so they both got a better reward.

 In a second set of experiments, the researchers trained pairs of rats to