.
>>
>>REVERSE- PARA- META- PSEUDO-RACIST BRAIN THEORY
>>
>>Copyright 2000 The Telegraph Group Limited
>>
>>SUNDAY TELEGRAPH(LONDON)
>>
>>November 19, 2000, Sunday
>>
>>
>>THE DIFFERENCE, a three-part series on genetics, begins on Channel 4
>>
>>tonight at 8pm.
>>
>>
>>He's got a better memory than us. New research suggests that one part of
>>
>>an aborigine's brain is 25 per cent bigger than a European's - but the
>>
>>academic community refuses to take it seriously, for fear of being
>>
>>branded 'racist'. ALASDAIR PALMER reports.
>>
>>
>>Sherilee is an eight year old who lives in Australia. She seems just
>>
>>like any other ordinary schoolgirl of her age, but she could help to
>>
>>resolve one of the most controversial topics in science: the
>>
>>relationship between genes and intelligence.
>>
>>
>>The question of how much of our brain power is fixed by what we inherit
>>
>>from our parents, and how much is a product of upbringing and education,
>>
>>
>>is one that appears to fascinate and frighten everyone - scientists
>>
>>included.
>>
>>
>>    It is not just the American Constitution that is framed around the
>>
>>conviction that we are all created equal. Practically the whole of
>>
>>contemporary politics is based on the idea that the differences between
>>
>>individuals are not fixed at birth.
>>
>>
>>    The suggestion that there are inherent differences, not just between
>>
>>individuals, but between races, is even less acceptable. There is now
>>
>>evidence, however, that one group of people may indeed have a superior
>>
>>mental capacity, in at least one respect, to everyone else - and some of
>>
>>
>>it comes from the eight-year-old Sherilee.
>>
>>
>>    Sherilee has an astonishingly accurate visual memory. She scores 100
>>
>>per cent on tests designed to measure how much individuals can remember
>>
>>of what they see. The only clue to the cause of her remarkable ability
>>
>>is her race: she is an aborigine, and aborigines have a proven ability
>>
>>to remember the exact location of objects that far exceeds that of other
>>
>>
>>ethnic groups. They can find their way across deserts, locate water
>>
>>holes and identify animal lairs with an uncanny accuracy. They also
>>
>>perform about 50 per cent better on visual memory
>>
>>tests than, for instance, Caucasians.
>>
>>
>>    What is the aborigines' secret? To some evolutionary psychologists,
>>
>>the answer is relatively straightforward. The aborigines were, for about
>>
>>
>>4,000 generations, or 80,000 years, hunter-gatherers in the deserts of
>>
>>Australia.
>>
>>
>>    That is enough time for natural selection to have worked on
>>
>>increasing the accuracy of aborigines' memory, because if you could not
>>
>>find your way through the desert, or to the waterhole, you would starve,
>>
>>
>>and so would your children. In the competition to stay alive, an
>>
>>accurate memory would - to put it mildly - have been an advantage.
>>
>>
>>    Are today's aborigine children the inheritors of that process? It has
>>
>>
>>certainly been speculated that their extraordinary visual memories are
>>
>>the result of genes selected over thousands of years by evolution.
>>
>>
>>    But Clive Harper, a professor of pathology in Sydney, may have
>>
>>discovered evidence that it is more than just a theoretical possibility.
>>
>>
>>He found that the visual cortex - the part of the brain used in
>>
>>processing and interpreting visual information - was about 25 per cent
>>
>>larger in aborigines than in Caucasians. He also found that they had
>>
>>many more nerve cells. That pronounced physical difference was almost
>>
>>certainly the result of different evolutionary
>>
>>pressures.
>>
>>
>>It is, as Prof Harper says, "difficult to prove that the greater number
>>
>>of nerve cells in the visual cortex is the secret of the aboriginals'
>>
>>phenomenal memories, especially when we know almost nothing about how
>>
>>the mechanism of memory works - other than that it involves the
>>
>>activation of nerve cells. Still, it is suggestive". It is "suggestive"
>>
>>enough to mean that Prof Harper could not get his findings published in
>>
>>any academic journal. His work, which he
>>
>>completed five years ago, was turned down because it was thought to be
>>
>>"racist".
>>
>>
>>    Science journal editors "were anxious", Prof Harper explains, "that
>>
>>this was going to be seen as some form of discrimination - which I was
>>
>>very disappointed about". Prof Harper was even refused permission to
>>
>>outline his findings at a conference in the United States. Even the
>>
>>original research that demonstrated the aborigine's superior memory
>>
>>skills has been buried.
>>
>>
>>    The cause of the anxiety was - and is - simple: the fear that the
>>
>>detection of any physical difference in the brains of different racial
>>
>>groups leads straight to Auschwitz. The idea that there are inherent,
>>
>>genetic differences between the different racial groups' mental
>>
>>abilities has about as bad a pedigree as it is possible to imagine.
>>
>>Hitler and the Nazis were obsessed by the idea, leading them to
>>
>>exterminate millions of Jews, gipsies and Russians on the grounds that
>>
>>they were "racially inferior".
>>
>>
>>    That fear is understandable in the light of the history of the 20th
>>
>>century, but it is chronically exaggerated. As scientists such as Prof
>>
>>Harper point out, an awareness that some groups have different
>>
>>capacities to others does not have to lead to a latter-day version of
>>
>>the extermination camps. It could, and should, produce education methods
>>
>>
>>that are better targeted at exploiting and developing the different
>>
>>abilities of different racial groups, helping
>>
>>to erode - rather than reinforce - their differing results within the
>>
>>education system.
>>
>>
>>    The anxiety that the mere mention of racial differences in capacities
>>
>>
>>is a first step that can lead only to the death camps has meant that
>>
>>many scientists prefer to turn away from evidence that mental abilities
>>
>>have been shaped by evolution rather than investigate it.
>>
>>
>>    One result has been that the field has been left to writers whose
>>
>>work has created a furore, leading to accusations that prejudice, not
>>
>>only science, played a role in determining their conclusions.
>>
>>
>>    Nearly eight years ago, Charles Murray and Richard Herrnstein
>>
>>published The Bell Curve. The book pointed out that Jews and Asians do
>>
>>significantly better on IQ tests than Caucasians. It highlighted the
>>
>>10-15 point gap between the average score that whites achieve on IQ
>>
>>tests and that achieved by blacks. The authors insisted that much of
>>
>>that gap was genetic: white people were generally inherently more
>>
>>intelligent than blacks.
>>
>>
>>    The Bell Curve generated a barrage of criticism, the most effective
>>
>>of which targeted the reliability of statistical methods that its
>>
>>authors used to reach their conclusions.
>>
>>
>>    Still, The Bell Curve is mild in comparison with Race, Evolution and
>>
>>Behaviour. That book, published by J. Philippe Rushton, professor of
>>
>>psychology at the University of Western Ontario in Canada, argues, among
>>
>>
>>other things, that it required greater intelligence to survive in the
>>
>>cold climates of Europe and Asia than in the tropical cornucopia of
>>
>>Africa, and also that there is an inverse correlation between penis size
>>
>>
>>and brain power.
>>
>>
>>    Prof Rushton cites what he claims is a battery of statistics to prove
>>
>>
>>that blacks have larger genitals and smaller brains than whites or
>>
>>Asians, and has summed up his thesis thus: "It's a trade-off. More brain
>>
>>
>>or more penis. You can't have both."
>>
>>
>>    Against that background, it may not be surprising that most
>>
>>geneticists would rather work on something less controversial than the
>>
>>genetics of intelligence. But the work is, nevertheless, going on.
>>
>>Robert Plomin and Peter McGuffin, of the Institute of Psychiatry in
>>
>>London, are two scientists who are trying to locate the genes for
>>
>>intelligence.
>>
>>
>>    Their investigation is not an easy task. About half of the 100,000
>>
>>genes in the human body affect the brain, and scores, possibly hundreds,
>>
>>
>>are involved in intelligence. Despite this, the researchers believe they
>>
>>
>>are making progress. "We have one very serious candidate for one of the
>>
>>genes involved in intelligence," says Dr McGuffin. "It's on chromosome
>>
>>six, and its called IGF 2R."
>>
>>
>>    Plomin and McGuffin, however, are not interested in the question of
>>
>>racial differences. Prof Plomin has said. "We have to look at the
>>
>>differences between individuals in intelligence to isolate genes for it,
>>
>>
>>not between groups." Even if they are right about IGF 2R, it will,
>>
>>according to Dr McGuffin, "probably be responsible for only about two
>>
>>per cent of the variations between individuals in intelligence. Our work
>>
>>
>>is, for the forseeable future,
>>
>>more of theoretical than practical interest," he said. "I don't see it
>>
>>as having any practical consequences."
>>
>>
>>    The hope that everyone is created equal is safe - for the moment.
>>
>>Protecting that hope appears to have become one of the central goals of
>>
>>scientific inquiry. The extraordinary abilities of Sherilee and other
>>
>>aborigines will remain tantalisingly "suggestive", but there are no
>>
>>plans to investigate further her, or anyone else's, unusual visual
>>
>>memory skills.
>>
>>
>>    "In terms of pursuing the studies of aborigines' brains," says Prof
>>
>>Harper, "I think that it is very unlikely that any work will be done in
>>
>>the future."
>
_____________________________________________________________________________________
Get more from the Web.  FREE MSN Explorer download : http://explorer.msn.com

Reply via email to