http://www.nytimes.com/2004/06/29/health/psychology/29bond.html

Addicted to Mother's Love: It's Biology, Stupid
By BENEDICT CAREY

Published: June 29, 2004

A mother's love is like a drug, psychologists say, a potent substance that
cements the parent-infant bond and has a profound impact on later
development.

But scientists have known little about how early mothering affects an infant
biologically. If it is like a drug, what kind of drug?

In at least one group of mammals, a team of Italian and French researchers
reported last week, it apparently acts like an opiate. Mice that lacked a
gene that lets them feel pain relief from opiates have severe difficulty
establishing bonds with their mothers, the researchers found.

When briefly separated from their mothers in the first week after birth - a
vulnerable period, when the babies can neither walk nor open their eyes -
the genetically altered babies did not cry out in distress nearly as often
as normal mice suffering the same separation. Cries for help are crucial to
cementing mother-child attachment, experts say.

The researchers tested to see whether the altered mice cried out in response
to other stresses like cold temperatures. They did. It was just separation
anxiety that they did not express strongly.

The cries "are part of an attachment behavior that maintains proximity
between infant and mother," said Dr. Francesca R. D'Amato of the CNR
Institute of Neuroscience in Rome and an author of the study, published in
the June 25 issue of Science. "It's fundamental to survival, but these
animals were not displaying it."

The study provides strong evidence that the same brain chemicals that
control physical pain also regulate the psychological ache of loss and
separation, she said.

It is also one of several recent experiments showing that alterations in a
single gene can radically reshape social behavior.

This month, scientists at Emory University in Atlanta reported that
injecting another rodent, the meadow vole, with a single gene turned
promiscuous males into stay-at-home dads. The gene helps the animals make
cellular receptors for a hormone, vasopressin, that is involved in social
bonding. Scientists had previously shown that rodents that were genetically
insensitive to another hormone, oxytocin, had difficulty forming pair bonds.
The neurobiology of mother-child attachment probably involves all three
systems in some way, scientists said.

"This latest study is the largest and best of its kind and provides very
strong evidence that maternal support has an opiate component," said Dr.
Jaak Panksepp, an emeritus professor of psychology at Bowling Green State
University in Ohio, who more than two decades ago was the first to propose
that opiate receptors were important in forming mother-child bonds.

Hormone and pain-relief systems work in similar ways in all mammals,
including humans.

The circulation of naturally occurring opiates like endorphins in the body
helps animals feel
relief and comfort. Messenger chemicals in the brain like dopamine then help
reinforce the sensation of being rewarded, whether from winning a bet,
meeting a potential mate or obtaining support from a parent.

Researchers say subtle variations in the genes that regulate these systems
could particularly interfere with the wordless emotion-based interactions
between a mother, or a primary caregiver, and child. Physical touch can
release opiates that have soothing effects, for example, but a baby with
reduced opiate sensitivity might not feel that relief so deeply. That could
in turn frustrate the mother or caregiver, who expects to provide comfort.

"What we may find, for instance, is that those individuals born with altered
opiate sensitivity will have a particular temperament, an inborn
psychological temperament, that would be difficult for a mother to connect
with," said Dr. Allan N. Schore, who studies attachment at the School of
Medicine at the University of California, Los Angeles. "The ability to feel
and express pain, to cry out and then feel comfort - these things tighten
the attachment bond. And that bond helps the child regulate their own
internal negative states as they grow."

Warm, attentive parenting can in fact help baby animals overcome some
genetic differences. In a series of experiments, scientists at McGill
University in Montreal have shown that baby rats repeatedly groomed, cuddled
and licked by their mothers grow up to be less anxious than those that
received less coddling. In a study appearing in the current issue of Nature
Neuroscience, the McGill researchers report that this physical mothering
early in life prompts long-lasting changes in the rats' genes that help the
animals manage stress throughout their lives.

Researchers at the National Institutes of Health have demonstrated a similar
effect in monkeys: Having parents that are warm and attentive protects young
animals from a specific genetic variation that would - in the absence of
such comfort and support - put them at high risk for aggressive, disruptive
behavior. These well-nurtured monkeys tend to become attentive parents
themselves: Their attachment to their mothers provides a model for the
relationships they will form much later with their own children.

"The important part of all this is that we're showing that an attentive
caregiver can actually alter the baby's genes, for the better," Dr. Schore
said.

A child with a lower genetic sensitivity to sensations of pain or pleasure
could thrive under the care of parent especially attuned to the child's more
subtle signals, he said, and their physiology might then correct or
compensate for the genetic difference.

Although scientists have much to learn about the many brain chemicals
involved in this process, some say it makes sense that they would include
opiates, a class of chemicals that includes habit-forming drugs like
morphine and heroin.

"Think about it: The connection to a parent is such an important one,
essential to survival," Dr. Panksepp said. "Wouldn't it make sense that this
social dependence is an addiction-type phenomenon?"
======================

The "fair use" doctrine allows limited reproduction of copyrighted works for
educational and research purposes. The relevant portion of the copyright
statue provides that the "fair use" of a copyrighted work, including
reproduction "for purposes such as criticism, news reporting, teaching
(including multiple copies for classroom use), scholarship, or research" is
not an infringement of copyright. U.S.C. Title 17, Chapter 1, Sec. 107.


Reply via email to