From: "R. A. Hettinga" <[EMAIL PROTECTED]>
Date: Fri, 25 Oct 2002 08:16:52 -0400
Subject: Eyewitnesses to Crime Often Blinded by Shock, Adrenaline

http://online.wsj.com/article_print/0,,SB1035490650107752191,00.html


The Wall Street Journal

October 25, 2002
SCIENCE JOURNAL
By SHARON BEGLEY

Eyewitnesses to Crime Often
Blinded by Shock, Adrenaline

The hundreds of detectives, police officers and federal agents
investigating the D.C. sniper slayings thought they finally had caught a
break last week. A couple of people saw a man shoot Linda Franklin, the
killer's ninth murder victim, in the parking lot of a Virginia shopping
center, a police spokesperson said.

But then the fallibility of eyewitnesses reared its ugly head. People who
said they saw the shooting offered such conflicting reports that the cops
couldn't produce a composite sketch. Only bank records, fingerprints and
other forensic data led them to arrest their two suspects Thursday.

That comes as no surprise to scientists who study eyewitness memory. As I
wrote last month in a column1 on false memories of the 9/11 attacks, even
memories of "unforgettable" experiences are a mishmash of first-hand
observation confounded by things we heard about, things we saw later,
things authorities suggested to us and things we imagined. But in addition
to these well-studied influences on memory, which exert their influence
after the actual observation, something else is going on: Sometimes we
don't see in the first place.

It was another murder that showed most poignantly the malleability of
eyewitness memory. Gerald Posner, author of the meticulously documented
"Case Closed," about the JFK assassination, recalls that during his
research for the 1993 book, he encountered "elaborate witness
recollections" of "second gunmen, puffs of smoke and mysterious federal
agents. In every instance, I went back to the witness's original statements
to the police, or to previously unshown television interviews of them that
day. The original statements are remarkable for their simplicity and
failure to have seen anything of importance."

The witnesses saw something that November day. But over time, says Mr.
Posner, "Many of them slowly added new elements to their stories, so that
after 25 years they had developed elaborate tales that bore absolutely no
resemblance to their contemporaneous statements."

While some people may have been seeking their 15 minutes of fame, Mr.
Posner believes: "Years of reading accounts in books and magazines,
watching television and film recreations, talking to other witnesses and
the like, filled in gaps in memory. They get to a point where they could
pass a lie-detector" test.

These are examples of post-event false memory. The National Institute of
Justice, the research arm of the Justice Department, now advises
investigators to be wary of all the ways eyewitnesses can unintentionally
confabulate. But while cops and prosecutors have gotten the word, juries
haven't.

"For juries, the single most important determinant of a witness's
credibility is his confidence, but we know that the most confident
witnesses can be the most wrong," says Saul Kassin, professor of psychology
at Williams College, Williamstown, Mass. One result: 80% of the 74
convictions later overturned by DNA evidence were obtained through mistaken
eyewitness IDs.

The leading edge of memory research now focuses on why observations don't
make it into synaptic storage in the first place. Thanks to an absence of
"encoding," there is nothing in the brain to recall. Why? The list of ways
we can fail to encode an observation is fairly dispiriting, but let me
summarize two 2001 experiments.

In one, two men entered a classroom filled with students -- one "the
culprit" and the other "the accomplice." The culprit, a white man, said he
had forgotten some slides. He picked up the carousel but dropped it,
creating a racket. When the students were later shown a photo lineup, they
were pretty good at identifying the culprit when his accomplice was also
white. But their accuracy plummeted when his accomplice was black.

"The students had a tendency to keep their eye on the black man," says
psychologist Stephen Goldinger of Arizona State University in Tempe, who
led the study. "A distinctive person receives more attention than a
'typical' person. This suggests that white culprits may be forgotten, or
mistaken for other white bystanders, if a black person is even present."

In another study, neuroscientists in London used brain imaging to
investigate cortical activity when people detect, or fail to detect, a
change in a scene. They found that unless the brain's attention area is
activated, people literally don't see the change. Photons striking the
retina aren't enough.

Adrenaline can also blind you. "A little arousal is good for memory, but
the effect looks like an inverted U: when adrenaline first increases,
memory improves, but as it keeps going up performance goes down," says
Prof. Goldinger. Memory also can be impaired by the presence of a weapon --
or any attention-getting object. If you see a robber handing a teller a
celery stick, memory for the robber's appearance stinks.

Which returns us to the sniper. "When you have a very upsetting event like
a woman being shot, what suffers is your memory for peripheral detail,"
says Elizabeth Loftus, professor of psychology at the University of
California, Irvine. Investigators, it seems, will have to build their case
on forensics.


Reply via email to