Delusional People See the World Through Their Mind's Eye


A mechanism for how the brain creates and maintains delusions is
revealed in a new study.

Having delusions, such as a belief in telekinesis, can influence how
people see the world - literally.

Human beliefs
<http://www.livescience.com/16748-americans-beliefs-paranormal-infograph\
ic.html>  are shaped by perception, but the new research suggests
delusions — unfounded but tightly held beliefs — can turn the
tables and actually shape perception. People who are prone to forming
delusions may not correctly distinguish among different sensory inputs,
and may rely on these delusions to help make sense of the world, the
study finds. Typical delusions include paranoid ideas or inflated ideas
about oneself.

"Beliefs form in order to minimize our surprise about the world," said
neuroscientist Phil Corlett of Yale University in New Haven, Conn., who
was not involved in the study. "Our expectations override what we
actually see," Corlett added.





The prevailing thinking holds that people develop delusions
<http://www.livescience.com/5508-people-unsure-beliefs-close-minded.html\
>  to predict how events in their lives will occur — just as
Pavlov's dog learned to predict that the sound of a bell ringing meant
dinnertime was imminent. Humans update their beliefs when what they
predict doesn't match what they actually experience, Corlett said.

But delusions often appear to override the evidence of the senses. To
test this idea, German and Swedish researchers conducted behavioral and
neuroimaging experiments on healthy people who harbor delusions.

In one experiment, volunteers were given a questionnaire designed to
measure delusional beliefs. Questions included: Do you ever feel as if
people are
reading your mind?; Do you ever feel as if there is a conspiracy against
you <http://www.livescience.com/11375-top-ten-conspiracy-theories.html>
?; Do you ever feel as if you are, or destined to be someone very
important?; and Are you often worried that your partner may be
unfaithful?

The participants then performed a task that tested their visual
perception: They were shown a sphere-shaped set of dots rotating in an
ambiguous direction, and asked to report which direction it was rotating
at various intervals.

People who harbored a greater number of delusional beliefs (those who
scored higher on the questionnaire) saw the dots appear to change
direction more often than the average person. The result confirms
findings from previous studies that delusional individuals have less
stable perceptions of the world.

In a second experiment, the volunteers were given glasses, which they
were told would bias their view so that the rotating dots would appear
to go in one direction
<http://www.livescience.com/14093-optical-illusions-gallery.html>  more
often than the other direction — a delusion, because these were
actually ordinary glasses. The volunteers performed a similar
dot-watching task, with a learning phase and a test phase. During the
learning phase, the dots clearly rotated in one direction, but during
the test phase, the direction was ambiguous.

While wearing the glasses, the volunteers reported seeing the dots
rotate in the biased direction, even during the test phase. They clung
to the delusion that the glasses altered their vision, even though the
visual evidence contradicted this idea, suggesting they used their
delusional beliefs to interpret what they were seeing.

A third experiment was similar to the second, but brain scans were taken
using functional magnetic resonance imaging. The scans showed that when
people were deluded about the direction of the dots' rotation, their
brains were encoding the delusion as if they had really seen the dots
move that way. In other words, people weren't just ignoring what they
saw; they were really seeing something else.

Furthermore, the brain scans revealed connections between a brain area
involved in beliefs, the orbitofrontal cortex, and an area involved in
visual processing, the visual cortex. (Both became active during the
delusional observations.)

Corlett finds the results exciting. The study "gives us a nice
explanation for the relation between belief and perception and how it
might go awry," he said.

But he cautioned that drawing inferences about people who are clinically
delusional, such as those with schizophrenia
<http://www.livescience.com/34794-schizophrenia-mental-disorder-percepti\
on-distortion.html> , may be premature. Time will tell whether the same
brain mechanisms are at play for these patients, he said.

http://www.livescience.com/39038-how-delusions-shape-perception.html
<http://www.livescience.com/39038-how-delusions-shape-perception.html>

Reply via email to