Important.

From: cingib <cin...@gmail.com>
Date: Tue, Mar 21, 2017 at 12:20 PM
Subject: Study: People Trust News Based on Who Shared it, Not on Who
Published It
To: Cynthia Gibson <cin...@gmail.com>

Avoiding articles from “the creep”: People trust news based on who shared
it, not on who published it
“When people see news from a person they trust, they are more likely to
think it got the facts right, contains diverse points of view, and is well
reported.”
By Laura Hazard Owen <http://www.niemanlab.org/author/lowen/>
@laurahazardowen <http://twitter.com/laurahazardowen> March 20, 2017, 12:12
p.m.

>From new mottos
<https://www.washingtonpost.com/opinions/mixed-feelings-on-democracy-dies-in-darkness/2017/03/03/ab33dbca-fea2-11e6-9b78-824ccab94435_story.html?utm_term=.72e429bd22eb>
to television advertising campaigns
<https://www.americanpressinstitute.org/publications/reports/survey-research/trust-news/>,
news organizations are refocusing efforts on why their readers should trust
them. But new research suggests they should also focus on who their
“ambassadors” are: The main factor in determining a reader’s trust in an
article appears to be who shared it, not the news organization that
published it, according to a study out Monday
<http://www.mediainsight.org/PDFs/Trust%20Social%20Media%20Experiments%202017/MediaInsight_Social%20Media%20Final.pdf>
from The Media Insight Project
<http://www.mediainsight.org/Pages/default.aspx>, a collaboration between
the American Press Institute and The Associated Press-NORC Center for
Public Affairs Research.

People *say* that the news organization matters to them a lot, and a 2016
study by the Media Insight Project
<http://www.mediainsight.org/Pages/a-new-understanding-what-makes-people-trust-and-rely-on-news.aspx>
found that Americans said the original news source was the biggest “cue”
<https://www.americanpressinstitute.org/publications/reports/survey-research/trust-news/>
they used to help determine whether they trusted the content in an article
they found on Facebook. “We wanted to test whether that was really true, or
whether people just believed that was the case.” So the researchers
designed a project:

We created a simulated Facebook post about health news and presented it to
an online sample of 1,489 U.S. adults who are part of AmeriSpeak, NORC’s
nationally representative survey panel.

Each person saw a health news post from one of eight public figures who
often share information about health, a list that ranged from Oprah and Dr.
Oz to the Surgeon General of the United States. Half the people were
randomly assigned a sharer they had earlier identified as a person they
trusted. The other half were randomly shown a sharer they had earlier said
they didn’t trust…

After viewing the post, everyone saw the accompanying health article
headlined: “Don’t let the scale fool you: Why you could still be at risk
for diabetes.” For half the sample, the article was labeled as coming from
The Associated Press (AP). For the other half, the reporting source was
fictional, something called the DailyNewsReview.com.

They found that a trusted sharer resulted in more trust for the article and
more engagement with the original news — including re-sharing the article
and signing up for news alerts from the source. Furthermore, the sharer of
the article affected how people thought about multiple facets of the
article: “When people see news from a person they trust, they are more
likely to think it got the facts right, contains diverse points of view,
and is well reported than if the same article is shared by someone they are
skeptical of.” (A respondent told the researchers in 2016: “I look who
shared it. If I have a friend that’s a creep I might not believe it. If a
friend is in a certain field, then I might believe what they post.”)

“People are often not aware of how much they are influenced by the identity
of the sharer,” the researchers write.

This study focused on adults, but its results echo the findings of a recent
study by the nonprofit Common Sense Media
<https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/2017_commonsense_newsandamericaskids.pdf>
on how 10- to 18-year-olds decide whether to trust news: Tweens and teens
are particularly trusting of news from family members and teachers.

There are some caveats here. The study only looked at the effect of
articles shared by public figures — in this case
<http://www.mediainsight.org/PDFs/Trust%20Social%20Media%20Experiments%202017/Social_Media_Experiment_Topline_2017.pdf>,
Oprah, celeb/trainer Jillian Michaels
<https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Jillian_Michaels>, NBC News’ Lester Holt
<https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Lester_Holt>, U.S. Surgeon General Vivek
Murthy <https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Vivek_Murthy>, CNN’s chief medical
correspondent Dr. Sanjay Gupta <https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Sanjay_Gupta>,
Dr. Oz <http://www.doctoroz.com/>, actress Gwyneth Paltrow, and
celeb/trainer Kayla Itsines <https://www.kaylaitsines.com/>. Results could
be different if they were applied to real-life friends in a Facebook feed.
Facebook readers may also already be more likely to see people they trust
in their feeds; they may un-follow friends who consistently share articles
they disagree with, or Facebook’s algorithm may not drive those friends’
shares to the tops of their feeds.

Still, the authors find implications for news organizations, news-literacy
advocates, and social networks:

*To publishers and journalists:* Your readers and followers are not just
consumers to monetize, instead they may be social ambassadors whose own
credibility with their friends affects your brand’s reputation. It is the
sharer’s credibility, more than your own, which determines other people’s
willingness to believe you and engage with you. This underscores the
importance of news organizations creating strong communities of followers
who evangelize the organization to others.

*To news-literacy advocates:* In light of growing concerns about “fake
news” spreading on social media, this experiment confirms that people make
little distinction between known and unknown (even made-up) sources when it
comes to trusting and sharing news. Even 19 percent of people who saw our
fictional news source would have been willing to recommend it to a friend.

*To Facebook and other social networks:* Facebook and other social networks
could do more to emphasize and provide information about the original
sources for news articles. The fact that only 2 in 10 people in our
experiment could recall the news reporting source accurately after seeing a
Facebook-style post suggests that basic brand awareness has a long way to
go. We found that sharers affect perceptions more than the original news
reporting source—but might that change if Facebook made the reporting
source label more prominent?

The study is here
<http://www.mediainsight.org/Pages/%27Who-Shared-It%27-How-Americans-Decide-What-News-to-Trust-on-Social-Media.aspx>
.
​http://www.niemanlab.org/2017/03/avoiding-articles-
from-the-creep-people-trust-news-based-on-who-shared-it-
not-on-who-published-it/?utm_content=buffer9f068&utm_
medium=social&utm_source=twitter.com&utm_campaign=buffer​

-- 
*Cynthia M. Gibson*

*)Twitter:  @cingib*

*www.cynthesisconsulting.com <http://www.cynthesisconsulting.com> *

――
View topic http://groups.dowire.org/r/topic/xpvGZVonMkYQrnPA5Chvk
Leave group mailto:newswire@groups.dowire.org?subject=Unsubscribe

Reply via email to