Sorry, one major error: in the 4th paragraph beginning, "For example," I
wrote "those
non-constraints on matter which Peirce calls 'habits'." The "non-"
shouldn't be there. GR

>>> Gary Richmond  12/11/11 3:05 PM >>>
Peter, Gary F., Jon, List,

I'm sorry it took a little while to respond to your message, Peter--the
end of the college term and personal matters took over (and continue to 
dominate my time)--which succinctly clarified your position.  

I agree with you that "the analogy [re: Peirce/Turing] is that Peirce
articulated a model of the mind which [. . .] is tacitly presupposed by
much of IA research." I hope we can discuss this model on the list, if
not this December, perhaps in the new year when the holidays have passed
and we, hopefully, all have a bit more time.

As to this Peircean model of mind, I would like to note in passing (for
now) that it seems to me that the self-same model of mind presupposing
IA research also influenced certain biosemioticians (for example, Eliseo
Fernandez, Soren Brier, and Terrence Deacon), this essentially semiotic
model being employed in their respective theories of emergence. 

For example, Fernandez argues that a top-down semiotic theory is needed
to complement the bottom up one of dominant biological theory, and Brier
that triadic semiotic theory complements and completes the dyadic code
semiotics of traditional scientific theory. Similarly, Deacon argues in
Incomplete Nature: How Mind Emerged from Matter, that a robust theory of
emergence will be frustrated until it rids itself of its residual
quasi-homuncular notions (while some biosemioticians simply ignore
anything smacking of 'teleology') and begins to deeply consider those
non-constraints on matter which Peirce calls 'habits'. The sub-title of
Deacon's new book, Incomplete Nature--again, highly recommended--might
more accurately be given as "How Mind Emerged from CONSTRAINTS on
Matter," the very Peircean Chapter 6 taking this up explicitly. But,
again, that's a discussion for another day. I'm pleased to learn, Gary
F., that you're reading Incomplete Nature and are interested in our
discussing it on list. I've sent copies as holiday gifts to several
friends, two of whom are members of peirce-l, and I'm hoping that they
too will want to participate in a discussion of some of the themes of
what I consider to be a most important work. Kalevi Kull, one of the
founders of biosemiotics, wrote that Incomplete Nature demonstrates how
some systems can be "alive and meaning making" (I'm not sure, yet,
whether or not he's overstating the case to say that with this inquiry
"the crux of life--and meaning--is solved" so that with it "the
twenty-first century can now really start"). 

On the related theme taken up in your second paragraph, you wrote:

PS: Engelbart's work - what I have read of it - deals primarily with the
machine side of the equation, and while Peirce anticipated some of what
Engelbart said, my chief claim is that Peirce's model of the mind
complements Engelbart's work. I discussed this with Engelbart fifteen
years ago, and he had never heard of Peirce before, but was not at all
dismissive of my claim. It is interesting, by the way, that Engelbart's
chief interest at that time - mid-nineteen-nineties - was to augment
group intelligence, reflecting an understanding of IA very much like
that articulated by Joe. I do not know whether he ever completed his
projected book on the subject.

GR: I met Engelbart about 5 or 6 year after you did, Peter, at the 9th
ICCS conference held at Stanford in 2001 (I was to attend all the
subsequent conferences through 2007). Several Peirce-influenced
researchers were involved in the conference: mathematicians, including
Rudolf Wille (Formal Concept Analysis) and Karl Erich Wolff, several
logicians, such as Joachim Hereth Correia (a principal contributor to
the recent strict mathematical proof of Peirce's 'reduction thesis') and
 including specialists in Peirce's Existential Graphs (EGs) such as
Frithjof Dau, and, of course, a large group, which included my good
friends Aldo de Moor, Harry Delugah, and Simon Polovina, centered around
the work of the logician John Sowa, the inventor of Conceptual Graphs
(CGs) which transmutes Peirce's EGs for contemporary, especially
electronic uses. 

I fondly remember having lunch with several of those just mentioned,
including Engelbart, which definitely left me with a sense that he'd
come to know Peirce's model of mind fairly well in those years since
you'd met him, and agree with you that he definitely felt it
complemented his own work in IA. (Btw, several Peirce-influenced
scholars--such as Terrence Deacon, Frederik Stjernfelt, Kelly Parker,
Christopher Hookway and myself included--were invited speakers at
subsequent ICCS conferences and, for a time at least, ICCS had, in part,
a decidedly Peircean flavor.)

You concluded:

PS: Engelbart's work - what I have read of it - deals primarily with the
machine side of the equation, and while Peirce anticipated some of what
Engelbart said, my chief claim is that Peirce's model of the mind
complements Engelbart's work. I discussed this with Engelbart fifteen
years ago, and he had never heard of Peirce before, but was not at all
dismissive of my claim. It is interesting, by the way, that Engelbart's
chief interest at that time - mid-nineteen-nineties - was to augment
group intelligence, reflecting an understanding of IA very much like
that articulated by Joe. I do not know whether he ever completed his
projected book on the subject.

GR: I don't believe he did finish that projected book, the only one I
know by him (there are several about him) being Boosting Our Collective
IQ, a collection of several key essays on the bootstrapping idea. For a
few years I was on the ba-unrev list centered around Engelbart's ongoing
research at the Doug Engelbar Institute he founded with his daughter,
now based at SRI. http://www.dougengelbart.org/ While "the machine side
of the equation" was certainly represented (for example, in the
discussions of dynamic knowledge repositories, or DKRs), there was, as I
recall, considerable discussion of his work in Collective IQ and CoDIAK
(acronym for how a group of people co- or *concurrently develop,
integrate, and apply their knowledge* to some end, goal, typically a 
project they share an interest in, strategically 'bootstrapping' it to
accelerate movement towards their goal).

I haven't kept up with Engelbart's work in recent years, but I think you
are correct in seeing it as "reflecting an understanding of IA very much
like that articulated by Joe."

Best,

Gary


Gary Richmond
Philosophy and Critical Thinking
Communication Studies
LaGuardia College of the City University of New York
E202-O
718 482-5700

*** *** *** ***
>>> "Skagestad, Peter"  12/04/11 11:29 AM >>>
Gary,

Just a couple of comments. In my view Peirce and Turing play analogous
roles in some respects but not in others. Turing directly influenced the
evolution of AI and articulated - whether in jest or in earnest - the
computational model of the mind later embraced and abandoned by Putnam,
articulated but never fully embraced by Fodor, and later advocated by
Haugeland, Dennett, and Pinker, among others. Peirce had no influence on
the evolution of IA (although he has influenced AI); the analogy is that
Peirce articulated a model of the mind which I have argued is tacitly
presupposed by much of IA research.

Engelbart's work - what I have read of it - deals primarily with the
machine side of the equation, and while Peirce anticipated some of what
Engelbart said, my chief claim is that Peirce's model of the mind
complements Engelbart's work. I discussed this with Engelbart fifteen
years ago, and he had never heard of Peirce before, but was not at all
dismissive of my claim. It is interesting, by the way, that Engelbart's
chief interest at that time - mid-nineteen-nineties - was to augment
group intelligence, reflecting an understanding of IA very much like
that articulated by Joe. I do not know whether he ever completed his
projected book on the subject.

Cheers,
Peter

________________________________________
From: Gary Richmond [richmon...@lagcc.cuny.edu]
Sent: Saturday, December 03, 2011 7:16 PM
To: PEIRCE-L@LISTSERV.IUPUI.EDU; Skagestad, Peter
Subject: Re: [peirce-l] SLOW READ: THE RELEVANCE OF PEIRCEAN SEMIOTIC TO
COMPUTATIONAL INTELLIGENCE AUGMENTATION

Peter, list,

I began my paper, "Trikonic Inter-Enterprise Architectonic,"
http://www.cspeirce.com/menu/library/aboutcsp/richmond/trikonic_architectonic.pdf
thus:

Peter Skagestad in “'The Mind's Machines: The Turing Machine, the Memex,
and the Personal Computer” considers the history of Artificial
Intelligence (AI) in relation to Intelligence Augmentation (IA) and
concludes that the American scientist, logician and philosopher, Charles
S. Peirce, provided a theoretical basis for IA analogous to Turing’s for
AI. Besides being keenly interested in the possibility of the evolution
of human consciousness as such, Peirce seems even to have anticipated
Doug Engelbart’s notion of the co-evolution of man and machine. In
another paper on ‘virtuality’ as a central concept in Peirce’s
pragmatism Skagestad goes so far as to suggest that “in Peirce's thought
. . . we find the most promising philosophical framework available for
the understanding and advancement of the project of augmenting human
intellect through the development and use of virtual technologies”  [GR:
a footnote here place reads: Skagestad notes, however, that for Peirce
“reasoning in the fullest sense of the word could not be represented
by an algorithm, but involved observation and experimentation as
essential ingredients"].

I have very much looked forward to this particular slow read. As you may
or may not know, I have been much influenced by especially those three
papers of yours on Arisbe to which you referred. Before I comment
further, is there anything in the above passage which you would say
needs correction or where you yourself have somewhat modified your
position?

Best,

Gary R.


Gary Richmond
Philosophy and Critical Thinking
Communication Studies
LaGuardia College of the City University of New York
E202-O
718 482-5700

*** *** *** ***
>>> "Skagestad, Peter"  12/03/11 11:56 AM >>>
I am now opening the slow read of Joe Ransdell’s paper ‘The Relevance of
Peircean Semiotic to Computational Intelligence Augmentation’, the final
paper in this slow read series. I realize that Steven’s slow read is
still in progress, but we have had overlapping reads before.

Since we are conducting these reads to commemorate Joe, I will open with
some personal reminiscences. In the fall of 1994, I bought the first
modem for my home computer, a Macintosh SE-30. At about the same time I
received a hand-written snail-mail letter from my erstwhile mentor the
psychologist Donald Campbell, who had just returned from Germany, where
he had met with Alfred Lange, who told him about an online discussion
group devoted to Peirce’s philosophy. Campbell was not himself very
interested in Peirce, but he knew I was, and so passed the information
along. And so I logged on to Peirce-L.

My connection was very primitive. I used a dial-up connection to U Mass
Lowell’s antiquated VAX computer, which I had to access in
terminal-emulation mode, whereby my Macintosh mimicked a dumb terminal
for the VAX, which ran the VMS (Virtual Memory System) operating system
and VMS Mail (later replaced with the somewhat more user-friendly
DECmail). It was extremely awkward to use, but it was free.

I had never met Joe Ransdell before * I only ever met him face to face
once * although we knew of each other’s work. Joe immediately caught on
to my difficulties in navigating VMS, and coached me patiently in the
technical side of things offline, while constantly prodding and
encouraging my participation in the online discussion. While never
leaving one in doubt of his own opinions, Joe consistently stimulated
and nurtured an open and critical, yet at the same time nonjudgmental
exchange of ideas and opinions. The intellectual environment Joe created
was an invaluable aid to me in developing my ideas on intelligence
augmentation and the relevance of Peircean semiotic thereto.

Now to the paper, available on the Arisbe site at
http://www.cspeirce.com/menu/library/aboutcsp/ransdell/ia.htm. It is the
longest paper in the slow read * 30 single-spaced pages plus notes * and
December tends to be a short month, as many listers will no doubt be too
busy with other things to pay much attention to Peirce-L in the final
week or so of the month. My feeling is that we will probably only be
able to hit the high points, but we will see how it goes. Since this is
the last slow read in the series, we can also go on into January, should
there be sufficient interest. I should add that the paper generated
considerable discussion on the list when Joe first posted it about a
decade ago; I do not know how many current listers were around at the
time, but I believe both Gary Richmond and Jon Awbrey took active part
in the discussion.

As I see it, the paper falls into four parts. The first part * roughly
one fourth of the paper * sets out the concept of computational
intelligence augmentation as articulated in three published papers of
mine, along with some reservations/revisions of Joe’s. The second part
adumbrates the Peircean/Deweyan conception of inquiry, the third part
examines Ginsparg’s publication system as a model of intelligence
augmentation, and the fourth part examines the role of peer review in
inquiry, sharply distinguishing editorially commissioned review from
what Joe understands proper peer review to consist in.

Personally, I shall naturally have most to say about the first part.
This does not mean that I think the list discussion ought to focus on
this part, at the expense of the other parts. This is decidedly not my
view. But given the attention Joe devotes to my work, I think the most
valuable contribution I personally can make here is commenting on, and
engaging in discussion on, what Joe has to say about my work.

I am not here going to rehash Joe’s admirable and scrupulously fair
recapitulation of my writings on intelligence augmentation * although
people may, of course, want to raise questions/comments about this or
that point in his recapitulation. What I propose to do in this initial
post is make a few introductory comments on intelligence augmentation,
offer my take on Joe’s differences with my articulation, and then
propose a few questions for list discussion * in full awareness that
other listers may find other questions to pose that may be as worthy or
worthier of discussion.

JR: “Peter Skagestad * philosopher and Peirce scholar * identifies two
distinct programming visions that have animated research into
computationally based intelligence which he labels, respectively, as:
“Artificial Intelligence” or “AI” and “Intelligence Augmentation” or
“AI”. The aim of the present paper is, first, to describe the
distinction between these two type of computational intelligence
research for the benefit of those who might not be accustomed to
recognizing these as co-ordinate parts of it, and then, second, to draw
attention to a special sort of Intelligence Augmentation (IA) research
which seems to me to warrant special emphasis and description, both
vbeause of its potential importance and because Skagestad”s account of
the distinctive features of IA research does not seem to me to capture
the most salient characteristics of this special part of it, perhaps
because it may not have occurred to him that it is distinctive enough to
require special attention in order to be recognized for what it is.”

PS: I’ll return to what I may have paid insufficient attention to and
why. First a little history. As far as I know, the concept of
intelligence augmentation was first articulated by Doug Engelbart in his
classic 1962 “Framework” report, where it denotes the use of computers
(or other artifacts) to augment human intellect by creating
human-computer systems whose behavior is more intelligent than that of
the unaided human. Engelbart acknowledges an affinity with the concept
of “intelligence amplification,” earlier articulated by the
cyberneticist W.R. Ashby. Based on my reading of Ashby, however, his
concept of intelligence amplification is broader and encompasses both AI
and Engelbart’s intelligence augmentation. Finally, the term
“intelligence amplification” was later embraced by the computer
scientist Frederic Brook, who used it much in the same sense as
Engelbart’s “intelligence augmentation,” and who, to the best of my
knowledge, was the first to use the abbreviation “IA” and explicitly
contrast it with “AI”.

Now, my thesis, advanced in three papers cited by Joe and available at
Arisbe, was that IA, as understood by Engelbart, presupposes a
conception of the mind as being exosomatically embodied, and that such a
conception, unbeknownst to Engelbart, had been articulated by Peirce,
and summarized in his dictum “all thought is in signs.” Joe does not
disagree with this, but does not think I go quite far enough:

JR: “In developing Skagestad’s conception further in the direction
indicated I also ground this in Peirce’s dictum, but I do so by making
explicit a different (but complementary) implication of the same
Peircean dictum, namely that all thought is dialogical. (JR’s emphasis)”

PS: A footnote indicates that I agree with this, which I do, but I want
to raise the question whether this implication is actually ever made
explicit by Peirce himself. Signs presuppose interpretation, and
interpretation presupposes interpreters, which is made very explicit by
Josiah Royce in his most Peircean writings, but did Peirce himself make
this explicit? I am not saying he did not, but I am curious about
references.

Joe goes on to make some valuable observations about the evolution of IA
that I had not made, to wit, that a great deal of what we now recognize
as IA, notably word processing, came about rather serendipitously,
because programmers needed to document their work and wanted to do so
without taking their hands off the keyboard. I have no argument with
that. I made the point that the emergence of the personal computer was
not a given consequence of the invention of the microprocessor, but also
required a particular vision of what computers were for. In so doing I
was simply rejecting technological determinism, not advancing any
monocausal thesis of my own.

I move on to what I take to be Joe’s most important reservation to my
treatment of IA:

JR: “I do not think that Skagestad has succeeded so far in identifying
precisely enough what it is that is fundamental in the IA tradition that
runs through Douglas Engelbart, J.C.R. Licklider, Ivan Sutherland, Ted
Nelson, Alan Kaye, * Tim Berners-Lee* That is, I do not find any place
where Skagestad describes IA in a way that seems to capture what the
various facets of it to which he appeals have in common. * My own hunch
*
and it is a little more than that, but it seems worth mentioning in a
suggestive spirit here * is that the key to the identity of  what
Skagestad characterizes as the IA tradition in computational research
lies in the conception of interactive computing*”

PS: I do not totally agree with Joe here. I gladly admit that I never
tried to identify what was fundamental to the IA tradition, believing
that job to have been already done by Engelbart. But interactive
computing, while essential to IA, has been endemic to computing of all
kinds during the past forty years. I played chess games with the MIT
computer as early as 1973; it was interactive, it had time sharing, but
there was nothing about it that specifically related to IA. I would
agree that collaborative computing is central to IA: more of that later.

Those are my initial thoughts on pages 1-8 of Joe’s paper. Some of it
was admittedly fast, as much of it is Joe’s recapitulation and as I see
it unproblematic exegesis of my papers. But others should feel free to
revisit any details I have skipped which may merit closer attention. I
will sit back now and let others weigh in.

Peter Skagestad





---------------------------------------------------------------------------------
You are receiving this message because you are subscribed to the
PEIRCE-L listserv.  To remove yourself from this list, send a message to
lists...@listserv.iupui.edu with the line "SIGNOFF PEIRCE-L" in the body
of the message.  To post a message to the list, send it to
PEIRCE-L@LISTSERV.IUPUI.EDU
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------
You are receiving this message because you are subscribed to the
PEIRCE-L listserv.  To remove yourself from this list, send a message to
lists...@listserv.iupui.edu with the line "SIGNOFF PEIRCE-L" in the body
of the message.  To post a message to the list, send it to
PEIRCE-L@LISTSERV.IUPUI.EDU

---------------------------------------------------------------------------------
You are receiving this message because you are subscribed to the
PEIRCE-L listserv.  To remove yourself from this list, send a message to
lists...@listserv.iupui.edu with the line "SIGNOFF PEIRCE-L" in the body
of the message.  To post a message to the list, send it to
PEIRCE-L@LISTSERV.IUPUI.EDU

---------------------------------------------------------------------------------
You are receiving this message because you are subscribed to the PEIRCE-L 
listserv.  To remove yourself from this list, send a message to 
lists...@listserv.iupui.edu with the line "SIGNOFF PEIRCE-L" in the body of the 
message.  To post a message to the list, send it to PEIRCE-L@LISTSERV.IUPUI.EDU

Reply via email to