Re: The difficulties of executing simple algorithms: why brains make mistakes computers don't.

2013-12-24 Thread Stathis Papaioannou
On 22 December 2013 23:28, Craig Weinberg  wrote:
> http://medicalxpress.com/news/2013-12-odd-easy-feat-mind.html
>
>> Even scientists are fond of thinking of the human brain as a computer,
>> following sets of rules to communicate, make decisions and find a meal.
>>
>> Almost all adults understand that it's the last digit—and only the last
>> digit —that determines whether a number is even, including participants in
>> Lupyan's study. But that didn't keep them from mistaking a number like 798
>> for odd.
>>
>> A significant minority of people, regardless of their formal education,
>> believe 400 is a better even number than 798, according to Lupyan, and also
>> systematically mistake numbers like 798 for odd. After all, it is mostly
>> odd, right?
>>
>> "Most of us would attribute an error like that to carelessness, or not
>> paying attention," says Lupyan, whose work was published recently in the
>> journal Cognition. "But some errors may appear more often because our brains
>> are not as well equipped to solve purely rule-based problems."
>>
>> Asked in experiments to sort numbers, shapes, and people into simple
>> categories like evens, triangles, and grandmothers, study subjects often
>> broke simple rules in favor of context.
>>
>> For example, when asked to consider a contest open only to grandmothers
>> and in which every eligible contestant had an equal chance of victory,
>> people tended to think that a 68-year old woman with 6 grandchildren was
>> more likely to win than a 39-year old woman with a newborn grandkid.
>>
>> "Even though people can articulate the rules, they can't help but be
>> influenced by perceptual details," Lupyan says. "Thinking of triangles tends
>> to involve thinking of typical, equilateral sorts of triangles. It is
>> difficult to focus on just the rules that make a shape a triangle,
>> regardless of what it looks like exactly."
>>
>> In many cases, eschewing rules is no big deal. In fact, it can be an
>> advantage in assessing the unfamiliar.
>>
>> "This serves us quite well," Lupyan says. "If something looks and walks
>> like a duck, chances are it's a duck."
>>
>> Unless it's a math test, where rules are absolutely necessary for success.
>> Thankfully, humans have learned to transcend their reliance on similarity.
>>
>> "After all, although some people may mistakenly think that 798 is an odd
>> number, not only can people follow such rules—though not always perfectly—we
>> are capable of building computers that can execute such rules perfectly,"
>> Lupyan says. "That itself required very precise, mathematical cognition. A
>> big question is where this ability comes from and why some people are better
>> at formal rules than other people."
>>
>> That question may be important to educators, who spend a great deal of
>> time teaching rules-based systems of math and science.
>> "Students approach learning with biases shaped both by evolution and
>> day-to-day experience," Lupyan says. "Rather than treating errors as
>> reflecting lack of knowledge or as inattention, trying to understand their
>> source may lead to new ways of teaching rule-based systems while making use
>> of the flexibility and creative problem solving at which humans excel."
>
>
>
>
> http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24156803
>
> The difficulties of executing simple algorithms: why brains make mistakes
> computers don't.
> Lupyan G.
>
> Abstract
>
> It is shown that educated adults routinely make errors in placing stimuli
> into familiar, well-defined categories such as triangle and odd number.
> Scalene triangles are often rejected as instances of triangles and 798 is
> categorized by some as an odd number. These patterns are observed both in
> timed and untimed tasks, hold for people who can fully express the necessary
> and sufficient conditions for category membership, and for individuals with
> varying levels of education. A sizeable minority of people believe that 400
> is more even than 798 and that an equilateral triangle is the most
> "trianglest" of triangles. Such beliefs predict how people instantiate other
> categories with necessary and sufficient conditions, e.g., grandmother. I
> argue that the distributed and graded nature of mental representations means
> that human algorithms, unlike conventional computer algorithms, only
> approximate rule-based classification and never fully abstract from the
> specifics of the input. This inp

Re: The difficulties of executing simple algorithms: why brains make mistakes computers don't.

2013-12-22 Thread Bruno Marchal


On 22 Dec 2013, at 13:28, Craig Weinberg wrote:


http://medicalxpress.com/news/2013-12-odd-easy-feat-mind.html

Even scientists are fond of thinking of the human brain as a  
computer, following sets of rules to communicate, make decisions and  
find a meal.



I thought that only Dreyfus cofused computer and expert system. A  
computer follows simple laws, but a brain, a priori, too. Sets of  
rules is ambiguous, and quite misleading when discussing the  
possibility or impossibility of computationalism.








Almost all adults understand that it's the last digit—and only the  
last digit —that determines whether a number is even, including  
participants in Lupyan's study. But that didn't keep them from  
mistaking a number like 798 for odd.


A significant minority of people, regardless of their formal  
education, believe 400 is a better even number than 798, according  
to Lupyan, and also systematically mistake numbers like 798 for odd.  
After all, it is mostly odd, right?



Well, that make sense if you say that a number is more "even" if it  
has a bigger power of two factor.


400 -> 200 -> 100 -> 50 -> 25  (biggest power of two factor = 16 = 2^4)
798 -> 399 (biggest power of two factor = 2 = 2^1)





"Most of us would attribute an error like that to carelessness, or  
not paying attention," says Lupyan, whose work was published  
recently in the journal Cognition. "But some errors may appear more  
often because our brains are not as well equipped to solve purely  
rule-based problems."


Nor is a digital neuronal net.




Asked in experiments to sort numbers, shapes, and people into simple  
categories like evens, triangles, and grandmothers, study subjects  
often broke simple rules in favor of context.


For example, when asked to consider a contest open only to  
grandmothers and in which every eligible contestant had an equal  
chance of victory, people tended to think that a 68-year old woman  
with 6 grandchildren was more likely to win than a 39-year old woman  
with a newborn grandkid.


"Even though people can articulate the rules, they can't help but be  
influenced by perceptual details," Lupyan says. "Thinking of  
triangles tends to involve thinking of typical, equilateral sorts of  
triangles. It is difficult to focus on just the rules that make a  
shape a triangle, regardless of what it looks like exactly."


In many cases, eschewing rules is no big deal. In fact, it can be an  
advantage in assessing the unfamiliar.


"This serves us quite well," Lupyan says. "If something looks and  
walks like a duck, chances are it's a duck."


Unless it's a math test, where rules are absolutely necessary for  
success. Thankfully, humans have learned to transcend their reliance  
on similarity.


"After all, although some people may mistakenly think that 798 is an  
odd number, not only can people follow such rules—though not always  
perfectly—we are capable of building computers that can execute such  
rules perfectly," Lupyan says. "That itself required very precise,  
mathematical cognition. A big question is where this ability comes  
from and why some people are better at formal rules than other  
people."


That question may be important to educators, who spend a great deal  
of time teaching rules-based systems of math and science.
"Students approach learning with biases shaped both by evolution and  
day-to-day experience," Lupyan says. "Rather than treating errors as  
reflecting lack of knowledge or as inattention, trying to understand  
their source may lead to new ways of teaching rule-based systems  
while making use of the flexibility and creative problem solving at  
which humans excel."






Following, or not, rules, is a level dependent question. You can  
simulate with prolog (which is a universal system with rules) a  
neuronal nets (a universal system without rule), and vice versa.


Bruno







http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24156803

The difficulties of executing simple algorithms: why brains make  
mistakes computers don't.

Lupyan G.

Abstract

It is shown that educated adults routinely make errors in placing  
stimuli into familiar, well-defined categories such as triangle and  
odd number. Scalene triangles are often rejected as instances of  
triangles and 798 is categorized by some as an odd number. These  
patterns are observed both in timed and untimed tasks, hold for  
people who can fully express the necessary and sufficient conditions  
for category membership, and for individuals with varying levels of  
education. A sizeable minority of people believe that 400 is more  
even than 798 and that an equilateral triangle is the most  
"trianglest" of triangles. Such beliefs predict how people  
instantiate other categories with necessary and sufficient  
conditions, e.g., grandm

The difficulties of executing simple algorithms: why brains make mistakes computers don't.

2013-12-22 Thread Craig Weinberg
http://medicalxpress.com/news/2013-12-odd-easy-feat-mind.html

Even scientists are fond of thinking of the human brain as a computer, 
> following sets of rules to communicate, make decisions and find a meal.
>
> Almost all adults understand that it's the last digit—and only the last 
> digit —that determines whether a number is even, including participants in 
> Lupyan's study. But that didn't keep them from mistaking a number like 798 
> for odd.
>
> A significant minority of people, regardless of their formal education, 
> believe 400 is a better even number than 798, according to Lupyan, and also 
> systematically mistake numbers like 798 for odd. After all, it is mostly 
> odd, right?
>
> "Most of us would attribute an error like that to carelessness, or not 
> paying attention," says Lupyan, whose work was published recently in the 
> journal Cognition. "But some errors may appear more often because our 
> brains are not as well equipped to solve purely rule-based problems."
>
> Asked in experiments to sort numbers, shapes, and people into simple 
> categories like evens, triangles, and grandmothers, study subjects often 
> broke simple rules in favor of context.
>
> For example, when asked to consider a contest open only to grandmothers 
> and in which every eligible contestant had an equal chance of victory, 
> people tended to think that a 68-year old woman with 6 grandchildren was 
> more likely to win than a 39-year old woman with a newborn grandkid.
>
> "Even though people can articulate the rules, they can't help but be 
> influenced by perceptual details," Lupyan says. "Thinking of triangles 
> tends to involve thinking of typical, equilateral sorts of triangles. It is 
> difficult to focus on just the rules that make a shape a triangle, 
> regardless of what it looks like exactly."
>
> In many cases, eschewing rules is no big deal. In fact, it can be an 
> advantage in assessing the unfamiliar.
>
> "This serves us quite well," Lupyan says. "If something looks and walks 
> like a duck, chances are it's a duck."
>
> Unless it's a math test, where rules are absolutely necessary for success. 
> Thankfully, humans have learned to transcend their reliance on similarity.
>
> "After all, although some people may mistakenly think that 798 is an odd 
> number, not only can people follow such rules—though not always 
> perfectly—we are capable of building computers that can execute such rules 
> perfectly," Lupyan says. "That itself required very precise, mathematical 
> cognition. A big question is where this ability comes from and why some 
> people are better at formal rules than other people."
>
> That question may be important to educators, who spend a great deal of 
> time teaching rules-based systems of math and science.
> "Students approach learning with biases shaped both by evolution and 
> day-to-day experience," Lupyan says. "Rather than treating errors as 
> reflecting lack of knowledge or as inattention, trying to understand their 
> source may lead to new ways of teaching rule-based systems while making use 
> of the flexibility and creative problem solving at which humans excel."
>



http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24156803

The difficulties of executing simple algorithms: why brains make mistakes 
computers don't.
Lupyan G.

Abstract

It is shown that educated adults routinely make errors in placing stimuli 
into familiar, well-defined categories such as triangle and odd number. 
Scalene triangles are often rejected as instances of triangles and 798 is 
categorized by some as an odd number. These patterns are observed both in 
timed and untimed tasks, hold for people who can fully express the 
necessary and sufficient conditions for category membership, and for 
individuals with varying levels of education. A sizeable minority of people 
believe that 400 is more even than 798 and that an equilateral triangle is 
the most "trianglest" of triangles. Such beliefs predict how people 
instantiate other categories with necessary and sufficient conditions, 
e.g., grandmother. I argue that the distributed and graded nature of mental 
representations means that human algorithms, unlike conventional computer 
algorithms, only approximate rule-based classification and never fully 
abstract from the specifics of the input. This input-sensitivity is 
critical to obtaining the kind of cognitive flexibility at which humans 
excel, but comes at the cost of generally poor abilities to perform 
context-free computations. If human algorithms cannot be trusted to produce 
unfuzzy representations of odd numbers, triangles, and grandmothers, the 
idea that they can be trusted to do the heavy